Pedagogia
Enviado por lupiila • 18 de Abril de 2014 • 3.334 Palabras (14 Páginas) • 134 Visitas
Chemistry Teaching—Science or Alchemy?
1996 Brasted Lecture
A. H. Johnstone
Department of Chemistry, Glasgow University, Glasgow G12 8QQ, Scotland
Introduction: Is Chem-Ed Research?
Many of us who work in universities have two main
roles: as researcher and as teacher. The balance between
these varies from one institution to the other, and the approach
to these roles can be very different. Some people see
teaching as a chore which gets
in the way of research, while
others view their teaching as
an exciting, creative, but often
frustrating pursuit. Journals
such as this regularly carry articles
describing some frontier
of chemical research along
with articles about teaching
innovation. Where the two
types of communication differ
is in their overt theoretical
stance. The research papers are copiously referenced to
theories, held beliefs, hypotheses, and objective measurement
and seek to build on and extend what has been done
before. The teaching papers, on the other hand, are full of
assertions, homespun wisdom, and ingenuity, and lack measurement.
I am suggesting that the development of good teaching
and the pursuit of research have (or should have) essentially
the same structure. We need some discipline in our
work to give it a clear focus, to be efficient in time and effort,
and to have a direction that is more often right than
wrong. We also need transferable outcomes that all can
share, to prevent the constant reinvention of fire.
The bulk of this paper is an attempt to do the gathering
together of things we have all been aware of, perhaps
intuitively, and to provide a working model for new research
and development in chemical education. It is an attempt to
systematize what is known into a usable form, which might
save us from confusing our enthusiasms for those of our students
and which will help us to go with the learning process
rather than across it or even against it!
The model draws upon the work of psychologists, educationists,
artificial intelligence workers, and dealers in
common sense.
Constructing a Model of Learning
In common with all living things, we are victims of our
environment, informed by our senses and reactions. However,
we have mechanisms by which we reduce the torrent
of sensory stimuli to manageable proportions, attending to
what seems to be important, interesting, or sensational. In
other words, we have a filtration system that enables us to
ignore a large part of sensory information and focus upon
what we consider to matter. To try to attend to everything
would be an impossibility leading to confusion and breakdown.
We then have to ask how the filter works. It must be
driven by what we already know and understand. Our previous
knowledge, biases, prejudices, preferences, likes and
dislikes, and beliefs must all play a part. How else would
we anticipate and recognize the familiar or be caught out
by a surprise?
Although in any one country or culture much of this
will be held in common, each individual will have a unique
set of held knowledge and beliefs that mark us out as separate
people and personalities.
Not only do we sense selectively, we also add, from experience,
to our sensory information
and “fill out” an otherwise
incomplete sensory experience.
Take a look at Figure
1. Is it just a lot of meaningless
blots? Try turning the
page upside down. What now?
The image is poor, but its
meaning is clearly being
supplemented by what you already
know and “filled out” to
a meaningful thing.
Somewhere in our heads
is a vast store of experience
and knowledge, one function
of which is to activate and
control our perceptual filter.
Stop and give some thought to
the implication of this for
teaching and learning. You
may be the provider of stimuli
during teaching, but how does
the student filter what you provide? If essential previous
knowledge or concepts or language is missing, how will this
affect what your students take out of what you say? Will
they miss essentials and grasp peripherals? Will they remember
the bangs and pops of a demonstration and totally
fail to grasp what you were trying to teach? Will your clever
graphics, trying to convey a three-dimensional structure on
a flat computer screen, fail because the students are not familiar
with drawing conventions or are incapable of generating
three dimensions mentally from two-dimensional
stimuli or even of seeing “near” things far away and vice
versa (1)?
You may be the
provider of stimuli
during teaching, but
how does the
student filter what
you provide?
Figure 1. Meaningless blots?
Turn the page upside down and
look again.
Vol. 74 No. 3 March 1997 • Journal of Chemical Education 263
Chemical Education Today
Figure 2. Information processing model.
Let us move deeper into the model by considering what
happens to the stimuli and information we admit through
the filter. It is thought to pass into a working space (or working
memory) where it is held and manipulated before being
rejected or passed on for storage. This part of the processing
train has been thoroughly researched by workers such
as Baddeley (2) and has given rise to a much more complex
model than I am presenting here. Readers might want to
pursue this further, but a simplified version will suffice for
the present purpose.
The working space has two main functions. It is the conscious
part of the mind that is holding ideas and facts while
it thinks about them. It is a shared holding and thinking
space where new information coming through the filter consciously
interacts
with itself and
with information
drawn from longterm
memory store
in order to “make
sense” (Fig. 2).
H o w e v e r,
there is a drawback.
This working
space is of
limited capacity
and I have written
about this before
in
...